Adverse childhood experiences, such as abuse, neglect or poverty, are associated with increased morbidity and mortality in adulthood (1) – (3). Low socioeconomic status, either as a child or as an adult, has long been recognised as such a risk factor (4, 5). In children, severe stress has been linked to changes in brain structure, stress responsivity and immunity (5), as well as to increased risk-taking behaviours such as smoking and substance abuse in adulthood (6).
In recent years, research has examined whether – and if so how – telomeres might be one of the links between adverse childhood experiences and later health. Telomeres are made up of repetitive DNA sequences (7, 8). They form protective caps on the ends of chromosomes and shorten with each cell division (8). When the telomeres reach a critical length, the cell goes into apoptosis or senescence, a state in which it can no longer divide (8). The enzyme telomerase maintains the length of the telomeres in stem cells, but is unable to counteract the shortening of telomeres over time in somatic cells (8). Based on average telomere loss with aging, it is possible to estimate how many years «older» the cells of one group are compared to another.
Shorter telomeres have been linked to increased morbidity and mortality (8), although the relationship is not conclusive (9). In addition to genetic variation, telomere length is strongly associated with age, race and gender, and to a lesser degree with body mass index (BMI), lifestyle and socioeconomic status (10). A number of studies have linked shorter telomeres with psychosocial stress in adults (11). This review summarises the research on the relationship between childhood stress and telomere length.
Method
A PubMed search using the search string «telomere AND (‘psychological stress’ OR ‘social environment’ OR socioeconomic OR abuse OR neglect OR ‘early life stress’)» generated 221 articles. Original studies published prior to 14 March 2015 in English or Scandinavian languages that referred to both childhood stress and measurement of telomere length were included. Studies were included on the basis of the title, but if it was unclear whether a study fulfilled the criteria, the abstract was also read. A total of 26 studies met the inclusion criteria for the review.
Results
The results are presented briefly in Table 1 (12) – (37), and are divided into four themes:
Studies that measured maternal stress in pregnancy
Studies that measured parental caregiving ability
The theme ‘childhood psychosocial stress’ included all studies that referred to various types of adverse experiences, such as physical and psychological abuse and neglect, or events such as divorce or loss of parents during childhood
Studies that mainly measured childhood socioeconomic status
Table 1
Overview of 26 original studies that examined the relationship between telomere length and various stressors from conception and throughout childhood and adolescence. In all studies, telomere length was measured at a single time point in adult men and women unless otherwise specified. PB = peripheral blood, PCR = polymerase chain reaction, UCB = umbilical cord blood, BM = buccal mucosa (mouth swab), SB = Southern blotting
First author |
Year and country |
Measure of stress |
Design/participants |
Tissue type/telomere measurement |
Controlled for |
Main findings |
Entringer (12) |
2011 Germany |
Severe stress in pregnancy |
Case control, N = 94; mother with stress (n = 45) and without stress (n = 49) |
PB PCR |
Age, gender, BMI, socioeconomic status, birth weight, childhood stress, disease, smoking, risk during pregnancy |
Severe stress was significantly associated with shorter telomere length in the children as young adults |
Entringer (13) |
2013 USA |
Pregnancy-specific stress during pregnancy |
Cross-section, N = 27, mother-newborn dyads |
UCB PCR |
Gestational age at birth, pregnancy complications, birth weight, gender |
Shorter telomere length in newborns was significantly associated with pregnancy-specific stress |
Asok (14) |
2013 USA |
Parental caregiving ability and responsivity |
Case control, N = 89 children at risk of abuse (n = 51) and not at risk (n = 38) |
BM PCR |
Socioeconomic status, birth weight, gender, minority background |
High-risk children had significantly shorter telomeres. Responsive parents moderated the association between telomere length and risk |
Brody (15) |
2015 USA |
Parental caregiving ability (supportive/non-supportive) |
Case control, N = 216 adolescents; Intervention (n = 114)/no intervention (n = 102) |
PB PCR |
Gender, socioeconomic status, current stress, smoking, alcohol consumption, BMI, blood pressure |
Shorter telomere length at the age of 22 years was significantly associated with non-supportive parenting style at the age of 17, but only in the control group |
Tyrka (22) |
2010 USA |
5 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cross-section, N = 31 |
PB PCR |
Age, smoking, BMI, hormonal contraception, minority background, socioeconomic status, current stress |
Adverse childhood experience was significantly associated with shorter telomere length |
Glass (20) |
2010 England |
3 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cohort, N = 1 871 |
PB SB |
Age, gender, smoking, BMI |
No relationship found between adverse childhood experiences and telomere length |
Kananen¹ (21) |
2010 Finland |
11 types of adverse childhood experience |
Case control, N = 974; with anxiety (n = 321)/control (n = 653) |
PB PCR |
Age, gender |
Adverse childhood experiences were significantly associated in a dose-response relationship with shorter telomere length in both groups |
Surtees¹ (25) |
2011 England |
8 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cross-section, N = 4 441 women |
PB PCR |
Health, socioeconomic status, overweight, smoking |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with increasing numbers of adverse childhood experiences |
O’Donovan (24) |
2011 USA |
5 types of adverse childhood experience |
Case control, N = 90; PTSD (n = 43)/control (n = 47) |
PB PCR |
Age |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with adverse childhood experiences in persons with post-traumatic stress disorder |
Kiecolt-Glaser (23) |
2011 USA |
11 types of adverse childhood experience |
Case control (subgroup), N = 86; care of individuals with dementia/controls |
PB SB |
Age, gender, BMI, caregiving responsibility |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with adverse events in childhood, but not with abuse, in both groups |
Savolainen (27) |
2014 Finland |
1 type of adverse childhood experience |
Cross section, N = 1 486 |
PB PCR |
Age, gender, sample quality (DNA), health, BMI, socioeconomic status (adult and childhood), smoking, physical activity, alcohol consumption, mother's age |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with adverse childhood experiences (evacuation as children) in persons with later traumatic experience in addition. |
Zalli (28) |
2014 England |
5 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cross section, N = 333 |
PB PCR |
Age, gender, socioeconomic satus, BMI |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with loss of mother as a child, but not with other types of adverse childhood experience |
Jodczyk (26) |
2014 New Zealand |
4 types of adverse childhood experience |
Birth cohort, N = 677 |
PB PCR |
Gender, minority, socioeconomic status at birth |
There was no association between telomere length and adverse childhood experience |
Tyrka¹ (29) |
2015 USA |
6 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cross section, N = 290 |
PB PCR |
Age, gender, socioeconomic status (adult and childhood), BMI |
Adverse childhood experience was significantly associated with shorter telomere length |
Drury (16) |
2012 Romania |
1 type of adverse childhood experience |
Randomised, controlled, N = 136 children; children's home (n = 68)/foster home (n = 68) |
BM PCR |
Intervention, gender, birth weight, age |
Shorter telomere length at the age of 6 – 10 years was significantly correlated with adverse childhood experience (time spent in orphanage before the age of four) |
Shalev (17) |
2013 England |
3 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cohort, N = 236 children |
BM PCR |
Telomere length at the age of 5 years, gender, socioeconomic status in childhood, BMI |
Significantly increased loss of telomere length at the age of 5 – 10 years was demonstrated in connection with experience of two or more adverse childhood experiences (various types of violence) |
Mitchell¹ (19) |
2014 USA |
5 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cohort (sub-group), N = 40 children |
BM PCR |
Age, minority, gender, BMI |
Shorter telomere lengths were significantly associated with adverse childhood experiences and low socioeconomic status |
Drury (18) |
2014 USA |
3 types of adverse childhood experience |
Cross section, N = 80 |
BM PCR |
Gender, age, mother's and father's age at conception, minority, socioeconomic status as child |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with adverse childhood experience, but only in girls in a dose-response relationship |
Adams (30) |
2006 England |
Socioeconomic status – occupational class of the head of the household |
Birth cohort, N = 318 |
PB PCR |
Smoking, alcohol consumption, BMI, diet, gender, father's age |
There was no association between socioeconomic status at birth (sole measurement) and telomere length as an adult |
Batty (31) |
2009 Scotland |
Socioeconomic status – height as adult |
Case control, N = 1 542 men with coronary disease (n = 484)/ without coronary disease (n = 1 058) |
PB PCR |
Age, smoking, BMI, alcohol intake, disease, statins |
No association between socioeconomic status as a child and telomere length |
Carroll (35) |
2013 USA |
Socioeconomic status – parents' educational level |
Cross section, N = 963 |
PB PCR |
Gender, age, minority, BMI, health, physical activity, smoking, diet. |
Longer telomere length was siginificantly associated in Latin-Americans with a father with a low education, not associated with mother's education. |
Kajantie (32) |
2012 Finland |
Soscioeconomic status – father's occupation (A, B), parents' educational level (C) |
Cohort, N = 2 376; 3 cohorts (A, B and C) |
PB PCR |
Age, gender, mother's age, socioeconomic status, smoking |
Socioeconomic status as a child was associated with a shorter telomere length in A and B (n = 2 142), not in C (n = 334). |
Robertson (34) |
2012 Scotland |
Socioeconomic status – parents' occupational class, insecure family economy, own car, subjective socioeconomic status |
Cohort, N = 2 185; born in the 1930s (A), 1950s (B) and 1970s (C) |
PB PCR |
Gender |
Shorter telomere length was associated with low socioeconomic status (parents' occupational class, own car) in childhood in C (n = 755), but not in A and B |
Cohen (36) |
2013 USA |
Socioeconomic status – parents owned home |
Cross section, N = 135 |
PB PCR |
Age, gender, minority, BMI, neuroticism |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with fewer childhood years (5 – 18) where parents did not own their own home |
Needham (33) |
2012 USA |
Socioeconomic status – parents' educational level and income |
Cross section, N = 70 children |
PB PCR |
Gender, age, minority, diet, physical activity, BMI |
Shorter telomere length significantly associated with parents with low education, not with income |
Theall (37) |
2013 USA |
Socioeconomic status – poverty and neighbourhood disorder |
Cross section, N = 99 |
BM PCR |
Gender, age, number of children in the family, socioeconomic status, number of years lived in neighbourhood |
Shorter telomere length was significantly associated with living in a neighbourhood with a high level of disorder and poverty |
[i] |
[i] ¹ Socioeconomic status has also been considered in these studies, but not primarily; see article text
There was a certain degree of overlap between the groups, which has been shown in the text and in Table 1.
Maternal stress in pregnancy
In the two studies identified (12, 13), stress – measured in terms either of adverse experiences (12) or of anxiety related to pregnancy and childbirth (13) – was associated with cells that were 3.5 years «older» in the children as adults in the one study (12), and was responsible for 25 % of the variance in telomere length in newborns in the other (13).
Parental caregiving ability
Poor parental caregiving ability was linked to shorter telomere length in children (14) and African American adolescents (15), but only when conditions around the children were worse than those of the control group. Children at high risk of abuse, determined by whether their parents were in contact with the child protection services, had shorter telomeres than children who were not at risk (14). In the high-risk group alone, good parental caregiving was associated with longer telomeres in the children. Brody et al. found that adolescents with non-supportive parents had shorter telomere lengths than those with supportive parents, but that this only applied to the group that had not participated in a behaviour modification programme for parents and adolescents (15).
Childhood psychosocial stress
A total of 14 studies examined the relationship between various types of childhood stress and telomere length either in children (16) – (19) or adults (20) – (29). The studies were mainly cross-sectional with retrospective reporting of childhood events. Adverse childhood experiences were recorded longitudinally in four studies (16, 17, 19, 26), and one of these also measured telomere length over time (17). Two studies found no relationship between childhood experiences and telomere length (20, 26), one of which measured life stress longitudinally in a birth cohort (26). The remaining 12 studies found an association between shorter telomere length and some or all of their measures of childhood psychosocial stress.
Three studies estimated the proportion of the variance in telomere length that could be attributed to psychosocial stress (16, 19, 27). The proportions were found to be 0.4 – 1.2 % (27), 12.2 % (16) and 40 % (19). A dose-response relationship between the number of stressors and telomere length as an adult was reported in three studies (21, 24, 25). One of these found that each «additional» stressor was associated with three «additional» years of ageing (25); the others did not present their results in this form.
All four studies that measured telomere length in children (16) – (19) found that psychosocial stress was associated with shorter telomere length. A larger proportion of life spent in an orphanage was associated with shorter telomeres in Romanian children who were followed longitudinally (16). Children who were exposed to two or more types of violence between the ages of 5 and 10 years showed significantly faster telomere shortening during this period than other children (17). Growing up in a family with difficulties such as parents in an unstable relationship or in prison, or family violence, was also associated with shorter telomere length in children (18, 19).
Childhood socioeconomic status
Twelve studies examined the relationship between childhood socioeconomic status and telomere length (19, 21, 25, 29) – (37), four of which are also mentioned above (19, 21, 25, 29). In several of these studies, large cohorts were followed with measuring of socioeconomic status in childhood and adulthood (Table 1). A multiethnic American study found that having a father with a low educational level was linked to greater telomere length (0.7 % of the variance), but only in Hispanics (35). Four of the studies (25, 29) – (31) found no relationship between selected measures of childhood socioeconomic status and telomere length as an adult. Three studies found that only some of their selected measures of socioeconomic status were associated with shorter telomeres as an adult (21, 32, 34). In a Scottish birth cohort from the 1970s, for example, growing up in a family from a low social class or without a car was linked to having cells that were almost 20 years and 8.5 years «older» respectively (34). However, the same study did not observe this association for other measures of childhood socioeconomic status, nor for cohorts born in the 1930s and 1950s (34).
Three studies measured socioeconomic status and telomere length in children (19, 33, 37). Children of parents with low educational levels had shorter telomeres, equivalent to six years of «additional» ageing, than children with at least one parent with higher education (33). Family financial status did not affect telomere length in this study. However, another study found that a doubling of family income was associated with 5 % longer telomeres, and that children of poorly educated mothers had telomeres that were around 30 % shorter than those of other children (19). In the third study, children from neighbourhoods with a high degree of disorder and poverty had below-average telomere lengths (37).
Discussion
Childhood conditions and telomere length
Studies of the association between maternal stress in pregnancy and parents’ caregiving ability and telomere length in the children have been too small and limited to enable definite conclusions to be drawn. The results are most consistent for psychosocial stress and telomere length. Twelve of fourteen studies show that different measures of such stress correlate with shorter telomere length in both adults and children. This is consistent with the conclusions in a review study of the subject from 2013 (38). Whether socioeconomic status in childhood is associated with shorter telomere length is more uncertain. Various measures of socioeconomic status have been investigated in the studies, and conflicting results have been arrived at.
Methodological problems
The studies in the review reveal a heterogeneity of measurement variables that extends beyond what our table presents. This makes it difficult to draw conclusions based on the studies viewed as a whole. The heterogeneity may be due to the fact that there is still no conceptual apparatus for defining which types of experience are definitively adverse. Any such apparatus would be problematic in itself, as experience is subjective and context-dependent. The study authors also report very different outcome measures for telomere length. This makes it difficult to compare the results of the studies, and impossible to conduct a meta-analysis.
The studies in the review have controlled for a number of factors found to be associated with telomere length, such as gender and age. However, one weakness is that the studies did not always control for the same factors.
In order to determine whether adverse childhood experiences lead to telomere attrition, more longitudinal studies are needed in which telomere length is measured over time. So far only one such study exists (17). As telomere shortening takes place slowly, it is recommended that change should be measured over several decades in longitudinal studies in order to achieve more reliable results (10). Moreover, experiences are by their very nature subjective phenomena. As such they are not quantifiable in the way that an objectively defined event is. At the same time, it is the subjects’ interpretation of their own experiences that presumably determines how these experiences affect their health subsequently. How to include these aspects in further research presents a challenge.
Telomere length is measured in different ways in some of the studies. Most have used cells from peripheral blood (leukocytes) and polymerase chain reaction tests (PCR), while a few have used Southern blotting (SB) (Table 1). Acceptable consistency has been found between the methods, but larger measurement error is associated with PCR. Cells from the buccal mucosa were used in a few studies (16), but there is still disagreement as to whether telomere length in different tissues can be compared (9, 40).
Possible mechanisms
There are several hypotheses about the effects of childhood stress on both telomeres and subsequent morbidity. There are probably many mechanisms involved. Behavioural changes due to growing up in a threatening and stressful environment, such as excessive threat vigilance, mistrust, poor social relationships and unhealthy lifestyles (41) may lead to an increased risk of morbidity. Smoking, overweight and limited physical activity are also associated with shorter telomere length (42), but not always conclusively.
Severe psychological stress in childhood leads to altered stress responsivity, both physiological and psychological (41). Psychological stress has been shown to correlate with higher cortisol levels, increased inflammation and increased oxidative stress (43). All of these factors are associated in their turn with shorter telomere length (43). Psychological stress can also accelerate ageing of the immune system through increased reactivation of latent viruses, and thereby increase wear and tear on T-cells (7). When telomeres in T-cells approach a critical length, the cells are no longer able to replicate, and in vitro these cells have been shown to secrete pro-inflammatory cytokines (7). A vicious circle may be created, where psychological stress causes increased cortisol production and oxidative stress, accelerated ageing of the immune system, shorter telomeres, increased inflammation and again shorter telomeres.
Conclusion
It may be possible to link severe stress in childhood with shorter telomeres. The evidence base is still too limited, recent and methodologically heterogeneous for it to be possible to assert this conclusively. More longitudinal studies are needed in which the participants are monitored closely, plus agreement as to what types of adversity should or can be measured, and how differences in telomere length can best be presented statistically. Having said that, a robust correlation has already been demonstrated between a tough childhood, poor parental ability to give care and low socioeconomic status on the one hand, and subsequent poor health on the other. The findings up to the present, including those from telomere research, give every reason to increase efforts in all health-related professions and society at large to prevent serious stress in children.
MAIN POINTS
An association is seen in many studies between adverse childhood conditions and shorter telomeres, although the evidence base is not conclusive.
The robust association between childhood adversity and impaired adult health may be mediated by reduced telomere length, among other factors.